The Conversation
  • Edition:
Available editionsClose menu
United States
  • Donate
  • Become an author
  • Sign up as a reader
  • Sign in
  • Get newsletter

Help The Conversation fight misinformation. Give now

The Conversation
Search
Academic rigor, journalistic flair
  1. Arts + Culture
  2. Economy + Business
  3. Education
  4. Environment + Energy
  5. Ethics + Religion
  6. Health + Medicine
  7. Politics + Society
  8. Science + Technology

It’s not what you say… it’s how you say it. Shutterstock

Here’s how your foreign accent can unfairly destroy your credibility

October 29, 2019 6.19am EDT
,

Author

  1. Monika Schmid

    Professor of Linguistics, University of Essex

Disclosure statement

Monika Schmid receives funding from the Dutch National Science Foundation NWO, the Dutch Royal Academy of Sciences KNAW and the Economic and Social Sciences Research Council ESRC.

Partners

University of Essex

University of Essex provides funding as a member of The Conversation UK.

View all partners

CC BY ND
We believe in the free flow of information
Republish our articles for free, online or in print, under a Creative Commons license.
  • Email
  • Twitter162
  • Facebook5.4k
  • LinkedIn
  • Print

There’s an old Punch magazine cartoon depicting a rather typical business boardroom. The group in the illustration includes one woman and several men, with the chairman saying, “That’s an excellent suggestion, Miss Triggs. Perhaps one of the men here would like to make it?”

The cartoon is still regularly shared on social media in the context of the debate around unconscious, or automatic, bias. This bias can happen when people unknowingly favour people who appear to be more like themselves, and discriminate against those who appear “different”. Examples include white people being more likely to find black male faces more threatening, or applicants with ethnic minority sounding names being less likely to get a job interview, even when they have the same qualifications.

Many employers now require their employees to take courses aimed at making them aware of this bias. Judging another person’s capability or credibility based on their gender, race or whether they use a wheelchair is obviously discriminatory, as such characteristics are unrelated to competence and expertise.

What accents say

But most people never stop to reflect on the degree to which accent can affect their opinion of the person speaking – particularly (but not only) if the accent gives the speaker away as someone who did not learn the language as a native speaker.

A particularly disturbing example was recently reported in the form of a judgment by Canadian judge Terry Clackson. The judgment lists in great detail a range of grammatical and phonetic “errors” committed by the Crown’s expert medical witness, Dr Bamidele Adeagbo – who is of Nigerian descent – rejecting the expert opinion delivered in his capacity as the physician who performed an autopsy that was a key element in the case.

In the present day and age, one would very much hope that stereotypes based on race, gender or sexual identity would not be seen as remotely acceptable arguments in a court ruling. So how can a supposedly imperfect command of the English language by someone who did not have the good fortune of being born and raised in what linguist David Crystal has termed an “inner circle” country – such as the UK, the US or Canada – be acceptable grounds for dismissing their considerable expertise?

‘You’re not from 'round here!’ Shutterstock

Like the non-male, the non-white and the non-able-bodied, non-native speakers often face an uphill struggle to be heard and taken seriously. Research shows that they are seen as less intelligent and competent, are less likely to be found suitable for higher-status jobs, and are less likely to be believed when delivering trivia statements such as “ants don’t sleep”.

How well we speak the language therefore is a measuring stick for how competent we are at anything else. As the linguist Vivian Cook pointed out, foreign language speakers are typically evaluated not on the basis of how far they have come, what they have achieved and how much they are able to do, but by the gap that still separates them from the “ideal” native. The enumeration by Justice Clackson of the imperfections in Adeagbo’s use of English is a textbook example of such an evaluation.

Such biases are particularly common in societies where it still is considered the norm for people to grow up speaking only one language, and where foreign language learning is deemed to be a “useless” luxury – although similar biases have been shown to apply among other non-natives who presumably should share the pain.

‘Where are you from?’

Even more disturbingly, labelling someone as “foreign”, with all the prejudices and stereotypes that this incurs, is something that literally happens within a split second. One study found that judgements on whether or not a speaker is a native can be surprisingly accurate upon hearing a speech segment that is just 30 milliseconds long.

Certainly, after a sentence or two, the listener will have made up their mind – and often will follow it up with the inevitable and ubiquitous question: “Oh, where are you from?” At that point, you know you have been labelled – and that your credibility is in tatters. What you say is now less important than how you say it.

While it is essential that these prejudices should be called out and recognised as part and parcel of unconscious bias, and that we should try to work against them, it is clear that this will have to be a marathon, not a sprint. The view that someone with a more “native” command of a language must be more knowledgeable than one who doesn’t – even if we are talking about a non-native who is considerably more expert – is deeply rooted and hard to challenge.

In an ideal world, society should treat everyone as equal, regardless of their wealth, power, status, race, gender – or accent. We do not live in such an ideal world. But there is a reason why many societies represent Lady Justice as wearing a blindfold: the judiciary must put facts and expertise in the balance and be able to abstract away from base prejudices.

“Thank you for your excellent analysis, Dr Adeagbo – perhaps a native speaker would care to present it?” is simply not good enough.

  • Discrimination
  • Linguistics
  • Accents
  • Unconscious bias
  • Tweet162
  • Share5.4k
  • Get newsletter Newsletter
Why The Conversation matters

Journalists and researchers have one thing in common: We seek the truth. I work with academics and scientists every day to communicate knowledge, discovery and facts to readers like you who also care about evidence over opinion. If you think this is important – and I’m confident you do – please help The Conversation grow with a gift in whatever amount you can afford. Thank you.

Martin LaMonica

Deputy Editor

You might also like

Cities with more black residents rely more on traffic tickets and fines for revenue

Your political views can predict how you pronounce certain words

Turing Test: why it still matters

Traitors, betrayal, surrender: British politics now dripping with terms that fuel division

Sign in to comment
14 Comments
Oldest Newest

Comments are open for 72 hours but may be closed early if there is a high risk of comments breaching our standards.

Show all comments

Most popular on The Conversation

  • Limited eating times could be a new way to fight obesity and diabetes
  • Why Americans are staying put, instead of moving to a new city or state
  • A quantum computing future is unlikely, due to random hardware errors
  • Remembering America’s lost buildings
  • Plague was around for millennia before epidemics took hold – and the way people lived might be what protected them
  • Courts have avoided refereeing between Congress and the president, but Trump may force them to wade in
  • Why your generic drugs may not be safe and the FDA may be too lax
  • Nicolas Bourbaki: The greatest mathematician who never was
  • Why it can be hard to stop eating even when you’re full: Some foods may be designed that way
  • What the Roman senate’s grovelling before emperors explains about GOP senators’ support for Trump

Expert Database

Want to write?

Write an article and join a growing community of more than 94,000 academics and researchers from 3,070 institutions.

Register now

  • Community standards
  • Republishing guidelines
  • Friends of The Conversation
  • Research and Expert Database
  • Analytics
  • Our feeds
  • Donate
  • Who we are
  • Our charter
  • Our team
  • Partners and funders
  • Resource for media
  • Contact us
  • En Español
Get our free daily newsletter
Privacy policy Terms and conditions Corrections

Copyright © 2010–2019, The Conversation US, Inc.

Support The Conversation

At The Conversation, scholars and editors work together to deliver rigorous yet accessible articles to readers who seek facts over lies. Now, every dollar up to $25,000 donated by readers like you will be gratefully matched by The Conversation’s Board of Trustees. All gifts will be counted and doubled, including monthly gifts. I am honored to support The Conversation and provide this challenge. Thank you.

W. Michael McKenna

McKenna and Partners | Member, The Conversation Board of Trustees