Background
According to epidemiologic studies that use recall of lifetime episodes, the prevalence of depression is increasing. This report from the Stirling County Study compares rates of current depression among representative samples of adults from a population in Atlantic Canada.
Methods
Sample sizes were 1003, 1201, and 1396 in 1952, 1970, and 1992, respectively. The depression component of the study's method, the DPAX (DP for depression and AX for anxiety), was employed. The original procedure (DPAX-1) was applied in all years. A revision (DPAX-2) was used in 1970 and 1992. The Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) was also used in 1992.
Results
With the DPAX-1, the overall prevalence of current depression was steady at 5% over the 2 early samples but declined in 1992 because of vernacular changes referring to dysphoria. The DPAX-2 gave a stable overall prevalence of 5% in the 2 recent samples, but indicated that women and younger people were at greater risk in 1992 than in 1970. The DIS, like the DPAX-2, found a current 1992 rate of 5% for major depressive episodes combined with dysthymia. Recalled lifetime rates using the DIS showed the same profile interpreted in other studies as suggesting an increase in depression over time.
Conclusions
Three samples over a 40-year period showed a stable current prevalence of depression using the DPAX methods that was comparable in 1992 with the current rates using the DIS. This casts doubt on the interpretation that depression is generally increasing. Within the overall steady rate observed in this study, historical change was a matter of redistribution by sex and age, with a higher rate among younger women being of recent origin.
THE EPIDEMIOLOGIC Catchment Area (ECA) program, the National Comorbidity Survey, the Edmonton Psychiatric Epidemiology Study in Canada, and the Lundby Study in Sweden suggest that rates of depression are increasing.1- 9 These studies used methods that ask subjects to recall episodes of depression and have been criticized for the fact that such memories may be unreliable.10- 13
This report takes a different approach, in that we focus on what subjects said about their current moods, symptoms, and impairment when interviewed as members of representative samples from the same general population at 3 points (1952, 1970, and 1992), thus providing a population perspective that makes long-term recall unnecessary. As in an earlier report that dealt with 1952 and 1970, we use the study's traditional methods for diagnosing disorders.14- 16 We added the Diagnostic Interview Schedule (DIS) of the ECA for the 1992 survey so that comparisons could be drawn with other studies.17
The research site, given the pseudonym Stirling County and located in Canada on the eastern side of the Gulf of Maine, was selected for its socioeconomic and cultural diversity.18- 20 The population numbered 19,989 in the early 1950s and 21,255 in the 1990s. As a region, it has experienced the social trends that characterize North America generally. These include the social advances of a rising standard of living, improved health care delivery, and increased educational opportunities. The changing times have also brought disadvantages, such as racial tension and increases in crime, including illegal drug use; loosening of family ties; and weakening of religious values. The economic picture has fluctuated, rising to a period of prosperity in the 1980s followed by a recent downturn. The primary industries have declined, large numbers of women have entered the labor force, the service and information industries have grown, and urbanism has increased through the media and travel.
The sampling procedures for 1952 and 1970 have been described previously.14,19,21 The following points are relevant to the sample of 1992: (1) Research maps and the census were updated and continued to provide the means for selecting and locating subjects. (2) The 1952 sample was a selection of heads of households, while the samples of 1970 and 1992 were selections of all adults 18 years and older, among whom heads of households were designated. (3) Communities of special interest because of socioeconomic and cultural contrasts were consistently oversampled using identical procedures. (4) For the remaining communities, the 1952 sample used variable sampling rates, the 1970 sample was based on selecting every ninth name from the census, and the 1992 sample used computer techniques to make a random selection from the census.
We have found that being a head of household was not related to the prevalence of depression and thus this report draws on all subjects with complete information (Table 1). Subjects gave informed consent to be interviewed. After the first survey, follow-up interviewing of subjects from earlier samples was coordinated with interviewing new subjects.22- 29 Because of this, interviewing for the second and third samples took longer than interviewing for the first sample. For the 1970 sample, interviewing extended from the summer of 1969 through 1971, and for the recent sample, from 1992 through 1995. Therefore, current point prevalence should be interpreted as referring to the disorders present at the time of interview.
Interview methods and definitions of depression
Two methods of data gathering and analysis employed herein were developed within the Stirling County Study and have been described elsewhere.15,16 They are named for the diagnostic focus on depression and anxiety (DPAX [DP for depression and AX for anxiety]), with the second (DPAX-2) being a revision of the first (DPAX-1) and with depression being separately analyzed in this report.
The original interview schedule used in the Stirling County Study contained only one question about dysphoria (using the word spirits to represent mood) and only one about functional impairment. According to psychometric principles, the greater the number of questions asked on a given theme, the greater the reliability.30 Thus, for the 1970 phase, several questions were added and they have been maintained in the subsequent issues of the schedule. By the 1990s, positive responses to the question about spirits had declined, while the proportion who responded positively to other questions about dysphoria ("feeling low and hopeless" and "wondering if anything is worthwhile anymore") increased. This suggested that phrases such as poor spirits or low spirits, like melancholy, were waning as familiar ways of talking about depressed mood. Thus, the DPAX-2 was prepared to make use of the new questions.
These methods employ a concept of depression that approximates the one used in the DSM-III and DSM-IV.31- 33 Both the original and revised versions follow an algorithm that takes into account "essential features," "associated symptoms," "impairment," and "duration."
For essential features, the DPAX-1 requires a response of sometimes or rarely to the question "Do you feel in good spirits?" responses that we will describe by the term poor spirits. The DPAX-2 requires a positive response to at least 1 of 3 questions (poor spirits, low and hopeless, or wondering if things are worthwhile).
For associated symptoms, the DPAX-2 needs positive evidence in each of the 3 clusters of symptoms concerning disturbances of appetite, sleep, and energy, but the DPAX-1 allows only 2 clusters to be represented if the subject is frequently in poor spirits. It is in the area of associated symptoms that both versions of the DPAX are limited in congruence to criteria spelled out in DSM-III and DSM-IV.
For functional impairment, both DPAX methods are similar to the model provided in DSM-IV, in which the following criterion was added: "The symptoms cause clinically significant distress or impairment in social, occupational, or other important areas of functioning."32(p327) The DPAX-1 requires a positive response to its one question about "going easy on work," while the DPAX-2 involves a larger array of questions. Two of them are general and deal with not being able to "get going" and not being "healthy enough to do things," while the others are linked specifically with depression and concern hospitalization and interference with work, family, and social activities.
Where duration is concerned, the DPAX-2 estimates the time of occurrence and the duration of the episode using supplemental questions, which are asked if positive evidence has been given about the essential features. These questions concern when the symptoms started, whether they still bother the subject, and, if not, when they stopped being bothersome. For the DPAX-1, time of occurrence is based on the period of going easy on work. Both algorithms require a minimum duration of 1 month, but the actual durations have been found to be longer. The average duration of essential symptoms found by the DPAX-2 varied between 10 and 11 years in 1970 and 1992; the average duration of impairment found by the DPAX-1 was about 8 years in each of the 3 surveys. This information does not mean that all aspects of essential features, associated symptoms, and impairment occurred together throughout these durations. Nevertheless, a sizable proportion of the DPAX depressive disorders seemed to be chronic.
The complete module for depression from the DIS, as used in the ECA, was added to the Stirling interview protocol for the 1990s.17 The purpose was to compare the DPAX findings with those of the ECA and other DIS studies that use DSM-III criteria, especially the other main North American investigation, the Edmonton Psychiatric Epidemiology Study.6,34,35 Major depressive episode (MDE) and dysthymia were used in combination for comparison with the DPAX, since the latter involves both acute and chronic disorders. One-month and lifetime rates of MDE were computed. One-month rates include episodes in existence at the time of interview or within the month preceding the interview. Lifetime rates include all episodes recalled as occurring at any time during the subject's life up to and including the time of interview. We followed the convention of the ECA in considering dysthymia to be a current disorder.36
The dependent variable for this study is the current prevalence of depression. Three explanatory variables are used: year of study, sex, and age. The main statistical procedure employed was hierarchical modeling, using a form of logistic regression known as log-linear analysis.37- 39 For analysis focused on between-year comparisons, year of study was investigated first in terms of a model that assessed its association with current depression controlling for age and sex, then by models that assessed the 2-way interactions of year of study with sex and age. The levels of significance were based on maximum likelihood χ2 statistics with appropriate degrees of freedom. To facilitate comparison with other investigations, within-year analysis was also carried out. Each of the 3 years was assessed individually for the associations of sex and age with current depression.
To adjust for variable sampling rates, we divided the county into 3 districts (2 for the oversampled and contrasting communities and 1 for the remainder of the county). The rates were standardized by the direct method to the pooled adult populations of the county for 1952, 1970, and 1992.40 The age profile of the population of Stirling County changed in ways similar to that for North America generally because of the increase of births after World War II. Since our goal was a comparison of rates over the years of study, we standardized the rates to the pooled population rather than choosing one year as opposed to another.
Estimates of current depression for three samples
The overall prevalence of depression according to the DPAX-1 remained stable at 5.3% in 1952 and 1970 but decreased significantly to 2.9% in 1992 (Table 2). The rates for women were somewhat higher than those for men in each year but the differences were not significant. Age was significantly related to the rates of depression in 1952 and 1970 but not in 1992. However, the patterns of association with age were not consistent over time or by sex.
The DPAX-2 indicated that the rate of depression was 5.3% in 1970 and 5.7% in 1992. In 1970, the prevalence of depression in men and women was not significantly different. In 1992, the rate for women was significantly higher than that for men. Age was significantly related to depression in 1970 but not in 1992.
The DIS results for 1992 indicated that the 1-month rate for MDE was 2.6%, while the rate for dysthymia without MDE was 2.9%; when these rates were combined, the current prevalence of acute and chronic depression was 5.5%. Women had a significantly higher prevalence of the combined disorders than men, but age was not significantly associated with the rate.
Agreement and disagreement between the dpax-1 and the dpax-2
To explore reasons why the 2 DPAX methods gave different rates in 1992 in contrast to the similarity of rates in 1970, we compared the results as they applied to individual cases. Agreement was better in 1970 than 1992 (Figure 1). To meet the criteria for essential features in the DPAX-2, at least 1 of 3 indicators of dysphoria was needed, including poor spirits. In the DPAX-1, poor spirits was the sole and necessary indicator of dysphoria. Therefore, if a subject fulfilled the criteria for essential features in the DPAX-1, that person automatically fulfilled the criteria in the DPAX-2. The obverse was not true; therefore, a main reason for disagreement was that a subject could exhibit dysphoria according to the DPAX-2 but not according to the DPAX-1 (Table 3).
Another contribution to disagreement was that only one question was available for disability in the DPAX-1, while the DPAX-2 has multiple indicators of disability. These 2 types of disagreements could be expected to lead to higher prevalence according to the DPAX-2, because it offered more options to describe mood and impairment. However, another type of disagreement constrained such a tendency—the DPAX-2 had more stringent criteria for associated symptoms than the DPAX-1.
The 2 DPAX methods produced similar prevalence rates in 1970 because positive responses about poor spirits had not yet declined seriously and because the number of subjects who did not meet the DPAX-1 criteria for poor spirits was about equal to the number who did not meet the DPAX-2 criteria for associated symptoms. The reason the 2 methods agree less well in 1992 relates primarily to the decreased use of poor spirits and secondarily to declining use of the concept of going easy on work as an indicator of impairment.
This comparison of the 2 DPAX methods suggests that the 1992 results based on the DPAX-1 are probably related to changing vernacular usage rather than to changing rates.
Comparison with other dis studies for mde
Comparisons based on using the DIS in 3 North American studies (ECA, Edmonton, and Stirling County) can be made for MDE as a separate disorder rather than in combination with dysthymia, as in the rest of this report.2,6,34- 36,41 Overall lifetime rates for MDE varied across the studies only from 6.3% to 8.6%, and 1-month rates from 2.2% to 2.6% (Table 4). In all comparisons, the rates for women were about 2 times higher than those for men. From both the lifetime and 1-month perspectives, younger people had higher prevalence than older people. In the Stirling County and Edmonton studies, the rates for the 2 younger age categories were more uniform than in the ECA study. However, the ratios comparing subjects younger than 45 years with those 65 years or older exhibit 2-fold to 4-fold differences.
Based on the evidence that the DPAX-1 findings for 1992 were influenced by vernacular changes, the most appropriate time comparisons from this study involve the DPAX-1 for 1952 and the DPAX-2 for 1970 and 1992, the latter being selected for the last 2 samples because its coverage of impairment is more congruent with the intent of the disability criterion of DSM-IV and because its requirement for 3 associated symptoms is a closer approximation of the 4 needed according to both DSM-III and DSM-IV.31,32 Using this selection of findings, it seemed worthwhile to compare current prevalence using a single threshold for age (<45 years vs ≥45 years). This threshold was chosen because the ECA findings of higher prevalence among those younger than 45 years have drawn considerable attention and influenced the design of the National Comorbidity Survey in terms of its focus on younger persons.2,4,41 Under these circumstances, the DPAX rates of depression in the 2 early samples were quite similar for women and men, and prevalence increased with age for both women and men (Figure 2). In 1992, the DPAX and DIS rates for women were similar and different from those in the earlier years. Both methods emphasized that the highest prevalence of depression was among younger women. The rates for younger men were similar across time and across method. The DPAX and DIS rates for older men differed, however, in that the DPAX showed the prevalence of depression rising with age, while it declined somewhat according to the DIS.
The conclusion we draw from this longitudinal analysis is that the current prevalence of depression in Stirling County remained steady at about 5% among successive samples of adults over the period from 1952 to 1992 and that these depressions tended to be chronic. At the end point of the study, the current prevalence of acute and chronic depression based on the DIS method was also about 5%.
At the same time, this study illustrates the problems posed for time-trend analysis by changes in the language people use to describe their moods and by the evolution of diagnostic criteria. There are 3 ways in which time can influence prevalence: show an increase, show a decrease, or maintain a stable rate. Each of these possibilities was, to some extent, evident. The DPAX-1 indicated a decrease by 1992, the DPAX-2 indicated stable prevalence, and the use of recalled lifetime evidence based on the DIS gave information of the same type that in other studies has been interpreted as showing an increase.
Our analysis of the DPAX-1 results indicated that the decrease in prevalence was influenced by changes in the vernacular and should therefore be discounted as showing a real decrease in prevalence. Where diagnostic standards are concerned, the methods used throughout this study share with contemporary approaches the concept that a diagnosis involves essential features, associated symptoms, duration, and impairment. At the level of several of the specific criteria, however, the DPAX and DIS methods differ, pointing to a dilemma for analyzing time trends. Use of the specific criteria of DSM-III, as with the DIS, depends on reconstructing the past by asking people to recall their psychiatric histories; such recollections may involve inaccuracies. On the other hand, data that do not require long-term memory, as in the use of current DPAX depression in samples dating back to 1952, cannot take into account all of the specific criteria that became standardized in DSM-III.
A question can be raised as to whether there is any common ground between the findings about a steady current prevalence of depression and findings that indicate increasing lifetime rates of depression. A stable current prevalence rate is not necessarily incompatible with increasing lifetime rates. If most of the disorders registered in the lifetime rates were short-lived, either because of their nature or because of effective treatment, their increase would have only small influence on current prevalence. Thus, at least theoretically, rising lifetime rates of acute depression and steady prevalence of chronic depression can occur together.
Increasingly, it is recognized that depression is an etiologically complex disorder that often runs a recurrent or chronic course.22,29,42- 44 In reporting the Stirling County findings comparing 1952 and 1970, we suggested that stable prevalence is more realistic than marked change for a type of disorder that exhibits such features.14 It seems unlikely, for example, that genetic and social circumstances would combine in a coordinated way during the same period to foster a pronounced increase or decrease. If changes were found to occur in a circumscribed historical period, it is likely that they would reflect social influences, but even where social experiences are concerned, there are cross-currents of advantages and disadvantages in a population at large that may give protection for some and have noxious effects for others. Thus, small changes in the distribution of depression may be more credible than wide fluctuations.
In considering these possibilities, the generalizability of the information from this study is an important issue. If the environment of the study area is unique and homogeneous, the findings would not have as much general relevance as if the region were typical of many others. Like most rural areas, this one has not grown much in population size, the way cities have, but its population is heterogeneous and its history has been molded by the same forces commonly observed throughout North America. In terms of psychiatric features, the DIS information from Stirling County in the present decade indicates that the prevalence of MDE is similar to what has been found elsewhere. This raises the possibility that other places might have appeared similar to Stirling County if the DPAX methods had been used. The population studies of similar vintage, such as the Lundby and Midtown Manhattan investigations, did not use structured diagnostic methods, thus making comparison problematic.8,45,46 Many of the other earlier studies used short inventories of symptoms with cases identified by a cutoff point rather than diagnostically oriented interviews.47- 51
While the methods used in this study (the DPAX and the DIS) show advances over the use of symptom inventories, both have limitations. It has been suggested that when the available methods for tracing trends are imperfect, it is useful to "pursue multiple pathways and look for agreements across them" (L. E. Robins, PhD, letter, March 3, 1998). Since 1980 most studies have employed either the DIS or the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI).52 The findings have been consistent in regard to 2 points. One is that the prevalence of MDE is about twice as high among women as men, and the other is that both lifetime and recent prevalence rates are higher among younger than older persons.1- 7,34- 36,41,53- 57 The results from the 1952 and 1970 samples in Stirling County were not congruent with these DIS and CIDI results. After 1970, however, the DPAX methods indicated that, within an overall stable rate, women were about twice as likely to report depression as men and that the burden of prevalence was moving toward younger women. In these terms, then, the DIS and DPAX methods provide mutual corroboration, although the DPAX findings point to the fact that the associations seen with consistency in DIS and CIDI studies may be of fairly recent origin.
This study suggests that interpretations about a general increase in depression should be tempered or modified in favor of a redistribution of prevalence by sex and age. The higher prevalence of depression among women younger than 45 years in the recent survey in Stirling County is especially worthy of attention. This change refers to a specific time span and a specific segment of the population. Therefore, it may be possible to develop testable hypotheses for investigation that would throw light on the origins of change, especially as these refer to the portion of prevalence that may be under the influence of social and historical circumstances.
Accepted for publication April 29, 1999.
This work was supported by grant MH39576 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Rockville, Md (Dr Murphy).
A preliminary version of this article was presented at grand rounds, Department of Psychiatry, Dalhousie University Faculty of Medicine, April 15, 1998, Halifax, Nova Scotia.
We thank Patricia Merritt, Barbara Burns, and Ellen Krystofik for field management and data processing; Ellen Krystofik and Barbara Burns for manuscript preparation; and Stephen C. Newman, MD, and Roger C. Bland, MB, for providing the lifetime and 1-month rates from the Edmonton study in the age categories used in this article.
Reprints: Jane M. Murphy, PhD, Department of Psychiatry, Room 9155, Massachusetts General Hospital, 149 13th St, Charlestown, MA 02129-2000 (e-mail: murphy@a1.mgh.harvard.edu).
1.Klerman
GLWeissman
MM Increasing rates of depression.
JAMA. 1989;2612229- 2235
Article 2.Robins
LNRegier
DA Psychiatric Disorders in America: The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study. New York, NY Free Press1991;
3.Cross-National Collaborative Group, The changing rate of major depression: cross-national comparisons.
JAMA. 1992;2683098- 3105
Article 4.Kessler
RCMcGonagle
KAZhao
SNelson
CBHughes
MEshleman
SWittchen
HUKendler
KS Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of
DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;518- 19
Article 5.Kessler
RCMcGonagle
KANelson
CBHughes
MSwartz
MBlazer
DG Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey, II: cohort effects.
J Affect Disord. 1994;3015- 26
Article 6.Bland
RCNewman
SCOrn
H Epidemiology of psychiatric disorders in Edmonton. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1988;3381- 80
7.Bland
RC Epidemiology of affective disorders: a review. Can J Psychiatry. 1997;42367- 377
8.Hagnell
OLanke
JRorsman
BÖjesjö
L Are we entering an age of melancholy? depressive illnesses in a prospective epidemiological study over 25 years: the Lundby Study, Sweden.
Psychol Med. 1982;12279- 289
Article 9.Grof
P The age of misery: affective disorders on the rise. Can J Psychiatry. 1997;42361- 362
10.Parker
G Are the lifetime prevalence estimates in the ECA study accurate?
Psychol Med. 1987;17275- 282
Article 11.Giuffra
LARisch
N Diminished recall and the cohort effect of major depression: a simulation study.
Psychol Med. 1994;24375- 383
Article 12.Simon
GEVonKorff
MUstun
TBGater
RGureje
OSartorius
N Is the lifetime risk of depression actually increasing?
J Clin Epidemiol. 1995;481109- 1118
Article 13.Angst
J Epidemiology of depression.
Psychopharmacology. 1992;106
(suppl)
S71- S74
Article 14.Murphy
JMSobol
AMNeff
RKOlivier
DCLeighton
AH Stability of prevalence: depression and anxiety disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1984;41990- 997
Article 15.Murphy
JMNeff
RKSobol
AMRice
JXOlivier
DC Computer diagnosis of depression and anxiety: the Stirling County Study.
Psychol Med. 1985;1599- 112
Article 16.Murphy
JMMonson
RRLaird
NMSobol
AMLeighton
AH Identifying depression and anxiety in a forty-year epidemiologic investigation: the Stirling County Study.
Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 1998;789- 109
Article 17.Robins
LNHelzer
JECroughan
JRatcliff
KS National Institute of Mental Health Diagnostic Interview Schedule: its history, characteristics, and validity.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38381- 389
Article 18.Leighton
AH My Name Is Legion: The Stirling County Study of Psychiatric Disorder and Sociocultural Environment. Vol 1 New York, NY Basic Books Inc1959;
19.Hughes
CCTremblay
MARapoport
RNLeighton
AH People of Cove and Woodlot: The Stirling County Study of Psychiatric Disorder and Sociocultural Environment. Vol 2 New York, NY Basic Books Inc1960;
20.Leighton
DCHarding
JSMacklin
DBMacmillan
AMLeighton
AH The Character of Danger: The Stirling County Study of Psychiatric Disorder and Sociocultural Environment. Vol 3 New York, NY Basic Books Inc1963;
21.Murphy
JM The Stirling County Study: then and now.
Int Rev Psychiatry. 1994;6329- 348
Article 22.Murphy
JMOlivier
DCSobol
AMMonson
RRLeighton
AH Diagnosis and outcome: depression and anxiety in a general population.
Psychol Med. 1986;16117- 126
Article 23.Murphy
JMMonson
RROlivier
DCSobol
AMLeighton
AH Affective disorders and mortality: a general population study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44473- 480
Article 24.Murphy
JMOlivier
DCMonson
RRSobol
AMLeighton
AH Incidence of depression and anxiety: the Stirling County Study.
Am J Public Health. 1988;78534- 540
Article 25.Murphy
JMSobol
AMOlivier
DCMonson
RRLeighton
AHPratt
LA Prodromes of depression and anxiety: the Stirling County Study.
Br J Psychiatry. 1989;155490- 495
Article 26.Murphy
JM Diagnostic comorbidity and symptom co-occurrence: the Stirling County Study. Maser
JDCloninger
CReds. Comorbidity of Mood and Anxiety Disorders. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1990;153- 176
27.Murphy
JMOlivier
DCMonson
RRSobol
AMFederman
EBLeighton
AH Depression and anxiety in relation to social status: a prospective epidemiologic study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48223- 229
Article 28.Murphy
JMMonson
RROlivier
DCZahner
GESobol
AMLeighton
AH Relations over time between psychiatric and somatic disorders: the Stirling County Study. Am J Epidemiol. 1992;13695- 105
29.Murphy
JM What happens to depressed men?
Harv Rev Psychiatry. 1995;347- 49
Article 30.Nunnally
JC Psychometric Theory. New York, NY McGraw-Hill Book Co1978;
31.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Third Edition Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1980;
32.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth Edition Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
33.Murphy
JM Continuities in community-based psychiatric epidemiology.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;371215- 1223
Article 34.Bland
RCOrn
HNewman
SC Lifetime prevalence of psychiatric disorders in Edmonton.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1988;33824- 32
Article 35.Bland
RCNewman
SCOrn
H Period prevalence of psychiatric disorders in Edmonton.
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1988;33833- 42
Article 36.Regier
DABoyd
JHBurke
JD
JrRae
DSMyers
JKKramer
MRobins
LNGeorge
LKKarno
MLocke
BZ One-month prevalence of mental disorders in the United States: based on five Epidemiologic Catchment Area sites.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45977- 986
Article 37.Everitt
BS The Analysis of Contingency Tables. London, England Chapman & Hall Ltd1977;
38.Laird
NOlivier
D Covariance analysis of censored survival data using log-linear analysis techniques.
J Am Stat Assoc. 1981;76231- 240
Article 39.Bishop
YMMFienberg
SEHolland
PW Discrete Multivariate Analysis: Theory and Practice. Cambridge, Mass MIT Press1975;
40.Anderson
AAuguier
AHauck
WWOakes
DVandaele
WWeisberg
HI Statistical Methods for Comparative Studies. New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1980;
41.Weissman
MMBruce
MLLeaf
PJFlorio
LPHolzer
C Affective disorders. Robins
LNRegier
DAeds. Psychiatric Disorders in America The Epidemiologic Catchment Area Study. New York, NY Free Press1991;53- 80
42.Klerman
GL Depression and related disorders of mood (affective disorders). Nicholi
AMed. The New Harvard Guide to Psychiatry. Cambridge, Mass Harvard University Press1988;309- 336
43.Sargeant
JKBruce
MLFlorio
LPWeissman
MM Factors associated with 1-year outcome of major depression in the community.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1990;47519- 526
Article 44.Judd
LL The clinical course of unipolar major depressive disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54989- 991
Article 45.Srole
LLangner
TSMichael
STOpler
MKRennie
TA Mental Health in the Metropolis: The Midtown Manhattan Study. New York, NY McGraw-Hill Book Co1962;
46.Srole
LFischer
AK The Midtown Manhattan Longitudinal Study vs ‘The Mental Paradise Lost' doctrine: a controversy joined.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1980;37209- 221
Article 47.Comstock
GWHelsing
KJ Symptoms of depression in two communities.
Psychol Med. 1976;6551- 563
Article 48.Finlay-Jones
RABurvill
PW The prevalence of minor psychiatric morbidity in the community.
Psychol Med. 1977;7475- 489
Article 49.Link
BDohrenwend
BP Formulation of hypotheses about the true prevalence of demoralization in the United States. Dohrenwend
BPDohrenwend
BSGould
MSLink
BNeugebauer
RWunsch-Hitzig
Reds. Mental Illness in the United States Epidemiologic Estimates. New York, NY Praeger Publishers1980;114- 132
50.Phillips
DL The ‘true prevalence' of mental illness in a New England state.
Community Ment Health J. 1966;235- 40
Article 51.Schwab
JJBell
RAWarheit
GJSchwab
RB Social Order and Mental Health: The Florida Health Study. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel Publishers1979;
52.Robins
LNWing
JWittchen
HUHelzer
JEBabor
TFBurke
JFarmer
AJablenski
APickens
RRegier
DASartorius
NTowle
LH The Composite International Diagnostic Interview: an epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;451069- 1077
Article 53.Canino
GJBird
HRShrout
PERubio-Stipec
MBravo
MMartinez
RSesman
MGuevara
LM The prevalence of specific psychiatric disorders in Puerto Rico.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44727- 735
Article 54.Wells
JEBushnell
JAHornblow
ARJoyce
PROakley-Browne
MA Christchurch Psychiatric Epidemiology Study, part I: methodology and lifetime prevalence for specific psychiatric disorders.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1989;23315- 326
Article 55.Offord
DRBoyle
MHCampbell
DGoering
PLin
EWong
MRacine
YA One-year prevalence of psychiatric disorder in Ontarians 15 to 64 years of age. Can J Psychiatry. 1996;41559- 563
56.Kessler
RCMcGonagle
KASwartz
MBlazer
DGNelson
CB Sex and depression in the National Comorbidity Survey, I: lifetime prevalence, chronicity and recurrence.
J Affect Disord. 1993;2985- 96
Article 57.Weissman
MMBland
RJoyce
PRNewman
SWells
JEWittchen
HU Sex differences in rates of depression: cross-national perspectives.
J Affect Disord. 1993;2977- 84
Article